After these scales have been introduced and explained, several possible
mechanisms by which the fighting arts may influence them will be discussed. As
the papers are reviewed these potential mechanisms will then be examined in
light of current research findings.
NATURE VS NURTURE
NATURE | NURTURE | ||
man the animal | tabula rasa | ||
body | brain | ||
|
|||
A | B | C | D |
At point B, man can modify his aggressive behavior but it is still innate and
latent. There is a certain feeling that aggression must be released in some way
but the method of expression can be selected.
At point C, man is seen as having the biological capacity for aggression, but
aggressive behaviours are learned. We have feet, fists, teeth, and perhaps a
certain urge for self-preservation, but we learn how to use these tools by
observing our fellow humans.
Finally, at point D, the idea of a biological basis for aggression is
rejected, and aggressive behaviors are strictly learned. This "societal
determinism" is every bit as inevitable and deterministic as is the
"biological fate" of point A.
Again, points A and D are derived from theoretical approaches while most
experimental discussions tend to fall somewhere along the line between them.
Russel Geen (1990) gives a model for aggressive behavior which depends on
background factors and immediate eliciting factors which combine to raise the
likelihood of an aggressive act. Inhibiting factors can then come into play to
modify or eliminate the aggressive impulse. Some of these background factors
include genetic makeup, sex, and personality (presumably the more permanent
portions of our learned behavior) while the immediate elicitors include stress
levels, general arousal, frustration or attack. Inhibitors and modifiers include
such things as fear of punishment or retaliation, and judgments of the other
person's intention when that person acted. Geen points out that the strictly
biological or the strictly learning-based explanations of aggression are really
just excuses. "It was my hormones" or "It was my bad
childhood" are both simply excuses which remove the responsibility for the
actions from the actor.
Karl (1991) also states that the idea of aggression as instinct is simply an
excuse for evil and there is in fact no "beast within".
Groebel and Hinde (1989) present a justification for the Seville Statement on
Violence, a declaration by several scientists in 1986. The statement claims that
it is scientifically incorrect to say that we have inherited a tendency to make
war from our animal ancestors; that war or any other violent behavior is
genetically programmed into our human nature; that there has been an
evolutionary selection for aggressive behavior more than for any other kind of
behavior; that humans have a 'violent brain'; and that war is caused by
'instinct' or any single motivation. This statement might be taken as some type
of proof that the biological argument for aggression is wrong, but in fact the
authors recognize that it is our biological bodies that determine what types of
aggression we can undertake. This statement does not, by any means, belong on
the extreme right hand side of our scale above.
Another book, edited by Silverberg and Gray (1991) and reviewed by Pellis
(1993), takes exception to the Seville Statement and argues that it does not
represent any kind of scientific consensus on the role of biology in human
violence, in fact it simply promotes the split between biological and social
sciences. Pellis points out that ignoring the biological bias in learning,
whereby some things tend to be easier to learn than others, would simply be
ignoring reality. The situation in the former Yugoslavia is given by Pellis as
an example of the failure of two generations of education in ethnic cooperation
within a period of months. Surely the children who grew up learning cooperation
could not have 'learned' to perform such barbaric atrocities within such a short
time without some sort of biological predisposition toward aggression. This
predisposition however, does not mean that there is a biological determinism.
Two theories on aggression which we encounter in the literature are those of
"cathartic" and "circular" aggression. The catharsis theory
views aggression as something like an instinct or drive which builds, rather
like the water behind a dam. In order for the water not to get so high as to
burst the dam and cause considerable damage, it must be "bled off" by
controlled spillways. Aggression can be "bled off" by violent sport or
other situations of controlled violence. This view of aggression would fall
somewhere between points A and B on our scale above. The circular theory of
aggression is well defined by the axiom "violence begats violence" and
simply states that aggressive actions provoke more aggressive actions which in
turn cause the original perpetrator to aggress further. This theory would fall
more toward the right hand end of the scale, between points C and D as
aggression is "learned" and used.
Groebel and Hinde (1989) define aggression as an interaction between two
individuals. "Attack on another individual usually involves risk of injury
for the attacker. It is therefore rarely single-minded, but is associated with
self-protective and withdrawal responses." (p.4) Other authors have
suggested that aggression may be directed toward inanimate objects as well.
Aggression may be of several types. Instrumental aggression is goal oriented and
occurs during theft or war. This may also be called felonious aggression.
Hostile or teasing aggression, or emotional aggression is directed toward
another and harm is intended. Defensive aggression occurs when one is attacked
or provoked. Games aggression occurs when one deliberately tries to injure
someone during a sporting venture. Dyssocial aggression is associated with gang
behavior, and bizarre aggression is due to psychopathic behavior. Violence is
defined by these authors as physical but not psychological damage to a person or
object.
These various attempts to classify aggression point out the variety of
opinions that can be expressed on the subject. Almost every author and
researcher will have an idea as to what aggression and violence is, and we now
present several scales which may help locate a particular definition. No one of
these scales will suffice of itself, to define an aggressive action. Each scale
may also be affected by other scales and these compound effects may make precise
definitions of aggression or violence most difficult.
The
recipient/observer can see both the action and the actor but must suppose the
actor's intent. The actor knows the intent, and acts on the recipient through
the action. To a much smaller degree the actor may be able to influence the
recipient/observer directly, perhaps by facial expression or other communication
apart from the action, and thus consciously influence the recipient/observer's
determination of intent.
In addition to these three factors, several other influences can be
identified which may affect the judgement of how aggressive is any particular
action. In all the scales below, a judgement of greater aggressiveness is
presumed for the left side of the scale, while a probable judgement of less
aggressiveness is represented to the right.
ACTION
MOST AGGRESSIVE | LEAST AGGRESSIVE | |||||
|
||||||
shooting | hitting | boxing | basketball | cards | sitting | sleeping |
FAST ACTIONS | SLOW ACTIONS | |||||
DELAYED REACTION | IMMEDIATE REACTION |
The speed of an action may represent its potential damaging effect, with a
fast swing of a stick at a friend being defined as more aggressive than a slow
one.
When responding to an attack, an action may be considered more aggressive
when it is delayed in time. It is not usually considered overly aggressive for
someone to hit back immediately on being hit, this is simple retaliation or self
defence and is done "in the heat of the moment". Hitting someone a
week later, however, is likely to be called vengeance or revenge and is likely
seen as overly aggressive. This is especially true when considered from a legal
viewpoint. This self defence aspect of the scale will be affected by other
factors, for instance, by judgements of the aggressiveness of the original
attack, the attributes of the retaliator and the effects of the retaliatory
action.
The effect of any particular action can often define its aggressive nature.
Hitting someone is likely considered a more aggressive act when death or
permanent damage results than when no bruises at all occur. Physical actions
with visible effects such as bruises are likely to be considered more aggressive
than the psychological damage caused by, for instance, taking away a possession.
An action with no effect at all, is unlikely to be labeled as highly aggressive.
EFFECT OF ACTION
GREAT DAMAGE | MINOR DAMAGE | TAKING A TOY | NO EFFECT | |
|
||||
death | permanent injury | broken bones | bruises | pain |
Attacking humans is not a single point on the scale, it is likely considered
less aggressive for a man to hit a stranger than it is for him to hit his wife
or children.
OBJECT OF ACTION
PEOPLE | ANIMALS | INANIMATE | ||||
|
||||||
wife | strange man | large eyed mammals | fish, frogs | trees, weeds | bacteria, virus | rocks |
ACCIDENTAL ACTIONS
AGGRESSIVE | NON-AGGRESSIVE | |||||
|
||||||
jumping on someone | theft | self defence | drunk driving | playing football | jumping around | fall on someone |
The fact that in many places the words used in the law are actually
"reasonable man" has led some to argue that there should be a
difference between what is permitted for a "reasonable man" and for a
"reasonable woman" and that women should, due to their more vulnerable
situation, be allowed more latitude in their justifiable defensive responses.
One of the ways to establish the intent of the actor is to consider the
following.
CLASSES OF AGGRESSION
HOSTILE | INSTRUMENTAL | DYSSOCIAL | DEFENSIVE | GAMES | BIZARRE |
|
|||||
intended damage | theft/war | football hooligans | fighting back | fouls | "madmen" |
Using the classification provided by Groebel and Hinde (1989), we can
construct an intent scale such as this one. Deliberately seeking to hurt someone
"for no reason" while in one's right mind is likely seen as extremely
hostile. If the actor hurts someone during a robbery, the damage may be the same
or greater but there was likely no intent to do the damage. The aggression may
be seen as less serious in this case. Similarly, running with a youth gang may
be seen as giving one less responsibility for the aggression. This reasoning is
more easily seen if one considers mob aggression in the crowd at a sporting
action. Hurting someone during a crowd melee is likely to be thought less
aggressive than hurting someone in the relative calm of a living room. Causing
injury while defending oneself from attack is certainly not an act which is as
aggressive as an unprovoked attack, even if the effect of the action is the
same. While hockey or football players who seek to damage their opponents are
said to be aggressive, they are not usually deemed as aggressive as a
wife-beater or a thief and they are not usually brought before the law. Finally,
one who is mentally ill is usually not thought to be aggressive in the same way
as is one who is sane. A deranged person is often more to be pitied than
condemned as aggressive. This last case may be subject to modification by
certain other factors. The various political and social forces at work in our
society may affect the judgement of how "crazy" a killer is, and of
how aggressive his or her acts. This judgement may be made independently of any
medical pronouncements on the mental state of the actors.
AGGRESSION VS ASSERTION
There is often a fine line between whether one is being aggressive or simply assertive. Speaking up, speaking up and poking a finger into someone's chest, and simply poking a finger into someone's chest are likely points along the assertion-aggression scale. A wide range of judgements can be made about the same action (poking a finger) depending on what the actor is saying or otherwise communicating at the time of the action.
AGGRESSION | ASSERTION | |||
|
||||
hitting with fist | poking with finger | poking and complaining | poking and explaining | explaining |
Along with the intent of the actor we must consider the various physical characteristics of the actor since these will almost always influence judgements of aggressiveness. A large, poorly dressed, unshaven male is often seen as inherently more aggressive than a small female child regardless of intent or the actual effects of an action. Questions of race and religion also enter into this scale. There will always be some groups that feel other groups are aggressive, simply by being other groups. This aspect may be treated more thoroughly in the power balance scale as it involves both the actor and the recipient or observer.
MEN | WOMEN | BOYS | GIRLS |
|
|||
"the other" | "those like us" | "us" |
We have not attempted to provide scales for the "reasons why" an
actor acts. These have been discussed in the previous section, and in several of
the scales given here, one can perceive potential reasons for acting, as for
instance, in cases of self defence.
There is an important distinction to be made between the recipient of the
action and the third party. It is a common finding in studies on sexual assault
that those women who meet the experimenter's definitions of having been
"raped", will deny it. While there seems to be no difference of
opinion as to what action occurred, there is a difference in the interpretation
of the meaning of that action. As a result, it is now common practice for
experimenters to ask questions such as "have you ever experienced forced or
attempted forced sexual intercourse against your will?" and to define this
as rape or attempted rape when reporting the results of the study (see Lori
Haskell and Melanie Randall, Toronto Globe and Mail Sept 9/ 1993 for an example
of this type of study). One reason for this difference of opinion could simply
be that experimenters are looking only at the action itself while the recipients
are looking at both the intent of the actor and at their own perceptions of the
action.
Perhaps one of our best clues as to whether we are being aggressed against is
the "fight or flight" response. This is the familiar churning
sensation that we feel in the stomach and is a biological reaction to many
environmental cues, most of which would indicate some danger to the organism. As
a learning animal, man can modify this "gut reaction" and even
eliminate it in situations where danger is known to exist. This is a very
important concept in most systems of fighting and is called variously, a cool
head, grace under fire, and in Japanese, fudoshin or immovable mind. The
opposite of this would be panic or a frozen mind (fushin in Japanese). If one is
exposed to a certain action and one does not experience this gut reaction, one
might be less likely to label it an aggressive act. On the other hand, if one is
stressed and anxious, a rather innocent action might trigger the physiological
reaction and one may call the action aggressive.
ALARM REACTION | CALM MIND | |||
|
||||
panic | anxiety | physical anticipation | calm | stress |
The degree to which the recipient is involved in the action or in the events
leading up to the action can influence the definition of aggression. If one is
struck when taking a massage, or playing a contact sport it is not likely to be
called aggression. If one is struck while standing on the sidelines of a
football game it is also not likely to be called aggression but if one is struck
from behind when walking down the street, even if accidentally, it is likely to
be thought of as an aggressive act. The closer or more linked one is to the
action, the less likely one is to define that action as aggression. There is a
large component of anticipation and preparation in this scale, what we expect,
doesn't alarm or shock us as much as what we do not expect.
RECIPIENT INVOLVEMENT
PASSERBY | SPECTATOR/BYSTANDER |
PARTICIPANT |
|
||
unprepared | somewhat prepared | prepared |
no responsibility | some responsibility | fully responsible |
LOCUS OF CONTROL
The way in which one views one's place in the world can affect one's
judgement of acts. A person with an external locus of control, someone who
believes themselves to be largely powerless in the face of external events, will
likely see many actions as being aggressive. After all the world is acting on
the individual and the individual has little control over those actions. At the
other end of the scale is the internal locus of control. A person who believes
that they have the power to influence the environment around themselves is also
less likely to judge actions as being aggressive, believing that they can
influence them, and even, perhaps, have some responsibility for them.
COPING METHOD
EXTERNAL | INTERNAL |
|
|
no control of environment | master of own fate |
acted on | acts upon |
Closely associated with locus of control, is the concept of perceived self
efficacy, the belief that one has the skills to influence the external
environment and that one can do it. This is often loosely termed self confidence
and this aspect of personality tends to be highly situation specific, as opposed
to the locus of control which is more of a method of looking at the world. If
one believes one has the skills to deal with a certain action, then the fear and
anxiety provoked by that action are reduced which could lead to the action being
judged less aggressive. To look at this another way, if one has low perceived
self-efficacy than even an innocent act may be interpreted as aggressive out of
fear caused by the lack of coping skills.
PERCEIVED SELF EFFICACY
LOW SELF EFFICACY | HIGH SELF EFFICACY |
|
|
can't cope | have coping skills |
STRESS LEVELS
Certain physical factors such as noise and light may act as general irritants and increase judgements of aggressiveness. In the same way, conditions of mental stress such as anxiety, sexual or emotional arousal or excitation might act as amplifiers of any judgements made. Depression or other conditions which would reduce the mental or physical responsiveness would also tend to reduce judgements of aggression.
HIGH | LOW |
|
|
stimulating environment | quiet environment |
physical excitation | sickness |
mental excitation | depression |
The pre-existing beliefs of a person can affect the judgement of an aggressive act. If a person believes "all men are rapists" or that "all feminists are castrating lesbians" or that "gang members are violent" than one can become either fearful of, or outraged at these groups (depending perhaps, on whether one has an external or internal locus of control). These prejudicial views may cause one to make an immediate judgement of aggression which might not be made by someone with a more open mind who might look at several other factors before judging.
HIGH BIAS | FLEXIBLE |
|
|
opinionated | open minded |
fearful | trusting |
outraged | understanding |
These scales are included with the recipient scales simply because both
observer and recipient must use many of the same cues regarding the action and
the actor's intent when forming an opinion of whether an act is aggressive or
not. A third party observer will also be influenced by the factors mentioned
above for the recipient but only as they apply to the observer, since a third
party can know nothing of the thought processes of the others. A third party
may, however, use his own mental processing as a reference more or less in
relationship to how similar he believes the recipient to be to himself. This
closeness will be affected by personal distance, the source of information about
the action, and the psychological distance from the recipient.
PERSONAL DISTANCE
NEARBY |
DISTANCED |
||
|
|||
me | my family | my friends | strangers |
in town | somewhere else |
INFORMATION SOURCE
NEARBY | DISTANCED | |||
|
||||
personal information | personal communication | television | radio | newspapers |
PSYCHOLOGICAL DISTANCE (THIRD PERSON)
We have mentioned the aspects of similarity in previous scales. An observer
is likely to be more harsh in the judgement of aggression when watching an
action against a recipient judged as similar, than against one who is dissimilar
to that third person. When considering a stranger, such aspects as sex and race
will likely give strong cues as to similarity, while social aspects such as
education and class should give less information and simply being in a situation
similar to one the observer has been in should give even fewer cues as to
similarity.
Third parties will also receive similarity cues in diminishing strength as the recipient characteristics get further from being human. Large eyed mammals look more like human children than do beady eyed rats, while plants, bacteria and rocks are not likely to remind anyone of anybody they know and are thus not likely to arouse much sympathy.
SIMILAR TO ME |
NOT SIMILAR |
||||
|
|||||
sex | race | nationality | education | economic class | situation |
humans | cute animals | ugly animals | bugs | plants | rocks |
When looking at a situation which is potentially aggressive, one thing that will likely be assessed is the relative power of the two parties. If the actor is judged more powerful than the recipient, any specific action is likely to be seen as more aggressive than if the power balance is tipped the other way. These power imbalances are not as clearly defined as they might at first appear. A case in point is the balance between black and white people. If the recipient or observer is afraid of blacks, then the greater power may be seen to be in their hands rather than in the hands of the whites, in which case the scale below would be reversed. Similar arguments can be made in the case of male and female, depending on the situation and on what is being defined as "power" in that situation. In the case of a child and an adult, the perceived power balance is likely to be as shown, as is the case of several actors and one recipient.
STRENGTH TOWARD ACTOR | BALANCE | STRENGTH TOWARD RECIPIENT |
|
||
male vs female | female vs male | |
white vs black | same qualities | black vs white |
adult vs child | child vs adult | |
many attackers | same numbers | one vs several |
There will always be an effect of the social sanction of violence or aggression on an individual's judgement. The most obvious source of information on how a society defines aggression is though the legal system. The particular situation in which a society exists at the time of the action will also affect the judgement of aggression. Acts that are tolerated and even seen as necessary during wartime, such as the internment of groups of people, may be labeled aggression during peacetime. State sponsored acts in some dictatorships may not be seen as aggression by the advantaged classes, while being labeled as such in more democratic societies. Some acts are also more likely to be labeled aggressive in relatively classless societies than in ones that are highly structured, especially when the acts are performed across classes. Finally, it is usually not considered aggressive for the state to execute an individual while it is always aggressive if an individual performs the same act.
LOW | HIGH SANCTION | |
|
||
peacetime | terrorist threat | wartime |
free society | pseudo-freedom | dictatorship |
egalitarian | pseudo-classes | class system |
individuals | vigilantes | state |
REALITY QUOTIENT
Finally, an act performed in the imagination with a war toy is not likely to
be considered as aggressive as when the exact same act is performed with the
real thing during wartime.
PHYSICAL REALITY | IMAGINATION | |||
|
||||
warfare | war toys | movies/theatre | storybooks | daydreams |
SOURCES OF AGGRESSION
If aggression is an innate, unchangeable, biological fate, than learning a
fighting art will have no affect on aggressive behavior at all. The more one
accepts that aggression can be learned, the more one must also accept that
aggression can be modified by learning. The relevant question then becomes 'what
affect does learning to fight have on aggression'. The acquisition of a set of
behaviors that have the potential to damage another person may or may not
increase the likelihood that those behaviors will be used. If the catharsis
theory of aggression is correct, than controlled aggressive behaviors such as
boxing or wrestling should reduce aggression outside the ring. If the circular
theory is correct, than learning a fighting art should create more aggressive
behavior.
If the fighting system studied also teaches things such as cooperation with a
partner or the rest of the class, control of emotions, control of actions, and
personal responsibility for actions, then it is possible that aggression may be
reduced in the student. Another consideration which must be answered by research
is the speed with which aggressive behavior can be modified by education. Will a
short course in boxing or Aikido change one's behavior one way or another, or
does this type of education require years to modify the habits of a lifetime?
Many authors suggest that the fighting arts, and other arts such as yoga,
promote an integration of the brain and the body, to develop a "bodymind".
Does this integration of the rational mind, (which may represent nurture or
learning), with the body, (representing nature or biology), have an effect on
aggressive or other behaviors?
If we use Geen's (1990) model of aggression we can consider what effect
learning the fighting arts has on the background factors, the immediate
elicitors and the modifying, external factors. It is unlikely that fighting arts
will have an effect on race or sex but they may have an effect on gender
orientation, on how "masculine or feminine" one is as judged by
various psychological measures of these things. Do the fighting arts have an
effect on "machismo"? It is also possible that the fighting arts may
affect the long term or stable "personality" of students. A more
likely place to examine the effects of learning to fight, would be on the
immediate elicitors to aggression. It is quite possible that effects might be
seen on such items as stress levels, general arousal, frustration, fear and the
perception of being attacked. It is also possible that learning fighting skills
may affect the inhibiting factors to aggressive acts, possibly by removing fears
of retaliation. For instance, the aggressor may gain increased confidence that
the person attacked cannot match the fighting skills learned, and so be more
likely to attack. The various substituant factors involved in aggressive acts
may also show changes through training in fighting arts.
ACTION SCALES
The particular aggressive acts performed, will not of course be changed by
learning a fighting art. A punch will remain a punch, but it is possible that
persons trained in the fighting arts may be able to very rapidly change from one
action to another. This fine motor skill could convert a punch to a push even
when only a few inches from contact. The actor may also be able to modify the
speed of the action, the effect of the action and thus, even the perceived
intent of the action. These near-immediate changes will depend of course on the
actor changing his mind and wanting to change from one act to another.
Training in fighting arts, with their emphasis on fast reaction time, is
likely to make any retaliatory act immediate rather than delayed, thus reducing
the chance of judgements that the action was "revenge" rather than
self defence. The influence of the timing of retaliatory acts has become more
important in recent years as lawyers have begun to use ideas such as
"battered wife syndrome" to argue self defence in cases where a woman
kills or maims her partner when she is under no apparent immediate physical
danger or threat. As mentioned before, the fine muscle control which is
presumably gained when learning a fighting art would allow an actor to control
the amount of damage inflicted by any particular act. This control would reduce
the likelihood that any act would have an effect that is not intended by the
actor, presumably reducing doubt about the aggression of the actor. On the other
hand, if the fighting art was taught in such a way that the student had no idea
of what type of damage he or she was capable of inflicting, the possibility
exists that the effect of an action would be out of proportion to the intended
damage. Such a situation might occur with throwing arts such as wrestling, judo
or aikido. Partners thrown in class know how to fall without damage while an
untrained person thrown in exactly the same way could be injured severely. In
all cases, however, it is quite probable that the effect of an act intended to
cause harm, would be more damaging from a trained person than from one not
trained in a fighting art.
The object of the action is not likely to be changed during the enactment of
a potentially aggressive act, but the choice of object might be influenced, as,
perhaps when a wall or door is struck instead of a person. It is hard to see,
however, how fighting arts might influence this choice except through the
practice (habit) of striking inanimate objects in arts like karate. Perhaps by
knowing how to strike inanimate objects without injury, the likelihood of
striking them rather than the desired (or eliciting) target might increase.
INTENT OF ACTOR
It is possible that learning a fighting art could modify the intent or at
least the apparent intent of the actor. Most fighting systems would train
students to be quite careful in their movements, and the more "self-defence
oriented" arts would presumably teach vigilance. This could very well mean
that the likelihood of an accident is reduced with the extra care taken when
moving around. This reduction in accidents would mean that any particular action
would more likely be interpreted as intentional. If the actor is not
"accident-prone" but is seen as being under good self control, then it
is less likely that any particular act would be judged an accident.
When considering the several "classes" of aggression, it is likely
that learning self defence oriented arts would change the likelihood of
"defensive aggression", probably by increasing it. Hostile,
instrumental and dyssocial aggression might be affected if the art included
ethical training in its curriculum but it is hard to see how the likelihood of
performing these aggressive acts would be affected directly by simply learning
how to fight. Similarly, aggression which occurs during a game might be affected
by ethical training (in, for instance, agonistic type fighting arts) but might
not be affected by learning the physical techniques themselves. The likelihood
of psychopathic aggression is not likely to be affected by learning a fighting
art unless these arts can be shown to have therapeutic value for this type of
mental aberration. On the question of aggression vs assertion, it is likely that
a fighting art that teaches self-control would make it unlikely that an
assertive act would be misinterpreted as aggressive. Training in fighting could
give a good appreciation for "personal space" and threat behaviors and
so might reduce the likelihood of an erroneous retaliatory act such as punching
the bank clerk who insists on getting another signature for something or other.
While it is not possible for any training to change the characteristics of
race, sex or other physical trait in an actor, it is possible that the actor
might be trained to a wider appreciation of who is "like us" and who
is "different". Many fighting systems are products of foreign cultures
that retain much of their original flavour. Training in these arts might widen
one's "family" to include those of other cultures and/or races.
PERCEPTION OF RECIPIENT OR OBSERVER
When looking at the scales involved with the perception of the recipient, we
must examine the effects of training in fighting arts on how the trained person
views actions received, with regard to both the judgement of aggression and on
the likelihood of retaliation to a perceived attack. Training in fighting arts
may also affect the judgement of a third party observer.
If a fighting art promotes the control of the alarm response, giving a
"cool head", then the physiological cues which might indicate an
aggressive attack will be absent from many situations. On the other hand, a
fighting art that emphasizes getting "psyched" for a competitive,
sporting match, could well provide trained cues to trigger the physiological
response which then might be tripped by the actor, leading to a more severe
judgement of aggression.
It is unlikely that training in a fighting art will affect a judgement of the
degree of participation in any particular act, but it may affect the assumption
of personal responsibility for that particular degree of participation. Training
that emphasizes the personal responsibility of each student for their own safety
and their own avoidance of conflict, could very well lead to a student assuming
that an act was in part "my own fault for being there" and lead to a
judgement of reduced responsibility and aggression from the actor.
It is uncertain whether the locus of control can be changed with education,
or how easily that could be done, but it is highly likely that the perceived
self efficacy of a student of the fighting arts will be changed by that
training. It is, after all, the skills to handle physical conflict that are
presumably being taught to students. Having the skills to handle a conflict, and
having the belief that one can use the skills could perhaps lead to a change in
the judgement of aggressiveness through, for example, a lowering of fear levels.
If the training acquired includes such features as breath control and other
commonly recognized methods of stress control, then the overall stress of the
recipient may be lowered, also lowering the "threshold of aggression
judgement".
Any educational experience is likely to open a "closed mind" and
training in combat arts is probably no exception. A reduction in prejudgement
about the actor's intent or actions will likely lower the chances of an
incorrect judgement of aggression.
As was mentioned above, training that includes acculturation to other
countries or other ethical systems may reduce the personal distance from the
observer to the receiver. This could lead to increased judgements of aggression
than might otherwise be the case as, perhaps, a stranger is now regarded as one
of the newly widened group or family. When training in a combat art, it may also
be easier for an observer to put himself in the place of a receiver of a
physical attack. This empathy may overcome the distancing effect of some
information sources and reduce the psychological distance between the observer
and the receiver, making for harsher judgements of aggression than before the
combat training.
On the other hand, combat training may affect the perception of power
balances. Those trained in self defence may well become less sympathetic to
recipients of physical actions, thinking "why didn't he defend
himself". It is possible that those who have experienced the power of a
trained woman might be less likely to accept automatically that women are less
powerful than men. It is also possible that the concentration on physical
conflict when learning a fighting art may influence judgements on such things as
the imbalance of power between persons of different colour. A white and a black
man may be judged solely on their physical attributes during a physical
altercation, and considerations of historical domination ignored.
Social sanction may be influenced by fighting arts through legal restrictions
on exotic weaponry, leading to a public perception that those who use
"martial arts weapons" are unusually aggressive. There is also the
possibility that a person's training may be taken into account during trials on
assault but on the whole, there is little indication that those trained in
fighting arts are treated any differently than untrained citizens.
A final, and very important matter which must be considered when examining
fighting arts training is their "reality quotient". Is training in
boxing "play fighting" or "aggression". There is plenty of
evidence that these two things are not the same and should be treated as
separate subjects when considering interpersonal relationships.
Michael Boulton (1991) states that not all behaviors that look aggressive are
aggressively motivated and separates "rough and tumble" fighting from
aggressive fighting. One interesting finding from his research is that 8 and 11
year old children could easily and reliably determine whether or not an act was
intended as an aggressive challenge, or an invitation to rough and tumble play.
An invitation to rough and tumble was usually met by a response in kind, while
acts of aggression were likely to be responded to by an act of aggression or by
no reaction at all. The judgement of aggressive behavior in the children was
made by the researchers on the basis of the same three major factors proposed
above, the characteristics of the action performed (action/outcome), the
presence/absence of signs of distress/annoyance of the recipient (perception of
recipient), and the presence/absence of signs of regret by the perpetrators of
injury/distress (intent of actor). Of special interest in this research was the
finding that less than 1% of bouts of rough and tumble play changed directly
into aggression.
This last point is important to remember when examining the psychological
effects of training in the fighting arts since it could easily be assumed that
what is being practiced in the classroom is the same as what occurs in the back
alleyway. If this were indeed the case, and if it is accepted that aggression
breeds aggression or violence breeds violence, than learning the fighting arts
should produce aggressive students. If, however, what is happening in combat
training is "rough and tumble play", than it may have little to do
with aggressive behavior at all.
In the next section we will examine the scholarly literature for evidence of any psychological effects of the fighting arts on the students.